Canny corvids curb cooperation after they’ve been cheated

October 28, 2015 • 9:30 am

Excuse the alliteration, as I’m tired this morning. But not too tired to report on a new paper in Nature Scientific Reports by Jorg Massen, Caroline Ritter, and Thomas Bugnyar (free access and pdf, reference below, popular summary at IFL Science). It shows something heretofore unknown in birds: a recognition that they’ve been cheated and a resulting reluctance to cooperate with anyone after that.

The paper begins with a useful literature survey of animals that cooperate with others (killer whales, Harris’s hawks, chimpanzees), animals that wait for another individual to arrive to help them perform a cooperative task (capuchins, bonobos, Asian elephants), and animals known to discriminate with respect to partners, choosing to work with those that have been more helpful (chimpanzees and coral trout [!]).

But that kind of partner discrimination hadn’t been described in birds. Now it is, thanks to a clever experiment in which ravens (Corvus corax), already known to be wicked smart, were forced into a situation where they had to cooperate to get food.

The experimental setup is shown below: ravens are put in a cage, and on the other side of the wire is a wooden platform containing two pieces of cheese. There are two strings threaded through grommets on the platform, and to pull the cheese toward the cage and get noms, two ravens have to pull at once; if only one pulls, the string simply slips out of the grommets and birds no can haz noms.

srep15021-f1
(From paper): Figure 1. Experimental set-up. Two birds have to pull the two ends of the string simultaneously to move the feeding platform in reach. If only one bird pulls, the string will just go through the two metal loops anchored to the feeding platform and become unthreaded, while the platform remains stationary. Picture drawn by Nadja Kavcik-Graumann.

There were two sets of experiments, involving in total 9 juvenile hand-reared ravens. The first conclusion is that ravens can’t figure out the task on their own; that is, they can’t spontaneously realize that there have to be two birds pulling together to get the noms. This was discerned from the failure of ravens to wait for the arrival of a partner before pulling the strings, and from their tendency to pull the string when there were no other birds present. As the authors note:

With regard to the control trials, in 243 out of 288 solitary trials (84.38%) individuals pulled the string even though this had no effect. Similarly, only 2 birds waited in 5 (out of 288: 1.73%) waiting trials for their partner to arrive at the apparatus and subsequently cooperated successfully. These data suggest that the ravens did not understand the need for a partner to solve the task in this experimental set-up.

So they’re not THAT smart!

However, the two experiments did give some intriguing results:

  • The ravens eventually did learn that joint pulling did get them noms. In the first study, using groups of ravens in the cage, there was spontaneous cooperation in 397 out of 600 trials, and every bird was successful at least 32 times.
  • Using knowledge about the age of the birds, their rank in the raven dominance hierarchy, sex, and kinship (relatives or not?), as well as “inter-individual tolerance” (the distance at which birds could tolerate each other’s presence when both were pulling on individual strings connected to cheese), the only predictor of cooperation was inter-individual tolerance (IIT). Here are the data, with the left graph showing the correlation between IIT and successful joint pulling when groups of birds were tested, and the right the same graph, but when only pairs of birds were tested. The correlation between IIT and joint success is significant, and shows that birds recognize each other, preferring to cooperate with those they can tolerate better:

Screen Shot 2015-10-28 at 7.11.51 AM

  • The experiment was repeated using pairs of birds in all possible combinations, except for those ravens at the top of the dominance hierarchy, who simply didn’t let any other ravens get the cheese. This experiment showed two things. First, some ravens cheated in the dyadic cooperation: after two of them pulled in the cheese bar, one would shove the other out of the way and eat both pieces of cheese. These were the CHEATERS. When the authors analyzed the results, they found that a raven was less likely to cooperate in subsequent trials if it had been victimized by a cheater. Here are those results with the caption:
Screen Shot 2015-10-28 at 7.17.12 AM
(a) Proportion of trials in which two birds cooperated successfully subsequent to an equal or unequal reward devision after the previous successful cooperation trial

As you see, the proportion of trials in which two birds worked together successfully was lower when the previous trial gave an unequal division of rewards compared to trials where both birds got the cheese (left bar compared to right). There was also an effect of kinship: kin cooperated significantly more than non-kin, but the p value wasn’t impressive (0.02), and the kinship effect wasn’t seen in the group experiment.

  • A final analysis tested the notion that, given the above results, cheaters who got two pieces of cheese would be even more likely to cooperate the next time. But that didn’t pan out: ravens who were cheaters, and got two rewards in a trial, were just as likely to successfully cooperate as those who got only one reward, as shown in the following plot. But again we find that if a raven gets no cheese, it’ll be less likely to cooperate in future trials:
Screen Shot 2015-10-28 at 7.29.58 AM
Proportion of trials in which a bird pulled the string after it had received zero, one or two rewards in the previous successful cooperation trial.
  • Finally, the authors tested whether cheaters became more fair—that is, allowed their pulling partner to get a piece of cheese—in subsequent trials. One might expect this if, because ravens cotton on to cheaters and don’t cooperate thereafter, the cheaters have to reinstate themselves in the esteem of their comrades by being fair. For if you’re marked as a cheater, nobody will cooperate with you and you might get less cheese in the future. Sadly, cheaters kept on cheating. I’m not sure if this is disadvantageous to them: after all, some birds get duped into cooperating with cheaters, even after they’ve been duped, so it may be generally advantageous to cheat.

Overall, then, this experiment shows that ravens can recognize individuals, have a tendency to cooperate with those whose presence they tolerate more easily, and can recognize cheaters and avoid cooperating with them in the future.

This is intriguing, but of course these are hand-reared ravens tested in a highly rarified experimental situation, so the relevance of the study to the behavior of these birds in nature is unclear. How often do ravens cooperate in the wild? Are flocks semi-permanent, so birds have a chance to recognize and avoid cheaters? Do flocks include relatives? These are questions that may already have an answer, but you’ll have to ask Bernd Heinrich.

Oh, I forgot: the experiment also shows that ravens love cheese. In a serious scientific omission, the authors don’t identify the type of cheese they used.

67_1ravens_ice_

h/t: Scott G.

________

Massen, J. G. M., C. Ritter, and T. Bugnyar. 2015. Tolerance and reward equity predict cooperation in ravens (Corvus corax). Nature Scientific Reports, 15021, doi:10.1038/srep15021

33 thoughts on “Canny corvids curb cooperation after they’ve been cheated

  1. I’m probably smarter than the average Corvid but couldn’t one really smart bird figure out that it could put the two ends of the string together and pull them both?

    1. From the diagram, that would require rethreading the strings through the grill so the ends are closer together.

  2. “Sadly, cheaters kept on cheating. I’m not sure if this is disadvantageous to them: after all, some birds get duped into cooperating with cheaters, even after they’ve been duped, so it may be generally advantageous to cheat.”

    The strategical null is tit-for-tat with slight cooperation, which is the best long term solitary strategy. [ https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Prisoner%27s_dilemma#Strategy_for_the_iterated_prisoners.27_dilemma ] If you don’t randomly try to cooperate with cheaters there can be a Nash equilibrium of mutual cheating, i.e. less than optimal loss.

    I assume there is a slight slice of an ‘optimal play’ population that can be cheaters and that will be successful at cheating at times, but it is possibly hurting them.

    1. Wouldn’t the success of cheaters depend on the population size? If the cheater continually cheats birds who have never been cheated before, they don’t have to worry about being caught. If cheating is an inherited trait, the cheaters would become a larger part of the population. Would this proportion reach an equilibrium?

  3. Cool. Did they let the ravens not in the experiment watch the ones doing it? If so, then an interesting follow-on would be to allow the ravens some ability to choose partners and see if they actively choose non-cheaters over cheaters.

  4. “….ravens who were cheaters, and got two rewards in a trial, were just as likely to successfully cooperate as those who got only one reward”

    I believe this is consistent with Game Theory results as well. The most successful game was where cheaters were punished but not forever.

  5. Very interesting, thanks for summarizing it. I question whether it’s worth worrying about the p-value in the kinship analysis, because as I’m sure you know, p-value is affected by sample size as well as effect size. The effect size with error bars and sample size stated would be a better way to judge the validity of the analysis.

    Corvids are very interesting birds. I believe ravens show cooperation in carrion feeding behavior, giving calls that carry a long distance to bring in other ravens to help before carrion feeders of other species can arrive.

    -jaxkayaker

  6. There’s an Aesop’s Fable about a raven (or crow, according to Wikipedia) and a piece of cheese – and a clever fox. I was reminded of it as I was reading.

  7. Evidently, the ravens learned how the device works by watching other ravens.

    This reminds me of a tropical bird called the brown booby that I’ve observed several times “surfing” the waves of air being pushed ahead of the ferry boats that travel between the islands of St. Thomas and St. John. The birds weave left and right across the bow, getting a free ride without any need for wing-flapping, until they spot a fish, then they dive after it.

    It occurred to me that this behavior couldn’t possible be an evolved instinct because power boats haven’t been around long enough for that, and since there’s no natural analogy that I can think of that’s even remotely similar, my conclusion is that somehow, perhaps a long time ago, a brown booby somehow discovered that it could soar in front of a fast-moving boat, and the knowledge spread from there.

    1. Soaring on updrafts certainly predates power boats. Such updrafts occur naturally in many situations, for instance where sea winds run into cliffs along the shore. The only new behavior here is recognizing moving boats as exploitable sources of updrafts.

  8. I’m not sure if this line at the end of the first paragraph is accurate:

    “It shows something heretofore unknown in birds: a recognition of cheaters and a refusal thereafter to cooperate with them.”

    It seems that those ravens who were cheated on were less likely to cooperate again in the future, regardless of which individual they were paired with. I may have missed it, but I didn’t see where the paper said that the cheated were less likely to cooperate with cheaters in the future, but rather that they were less likely to cooperate in general. It would be cool if we knew they recognized cheaters and penalized them.

  9. Thanks Prof Coyne for putting in the work for this post. I know you find the lack of comments on scientific posts troubling, but please keep doing them. It is hugely rewarding to learn about these sorts of studies.

  10. Cool, thanks for this write-up! I just downloaded the paper to read myself later (hopefully tonight)! I’m always fascinated to read about research in cognitive capacity and social dynamics in birds.

  11. What type of cheese was used? For the losers no doubt hard!

    I think EVERYTHING (almost!) is about avoiding cheaters. It is the great dilemma of societies – co-operation versus selfishness.

  12. Saw film of this type of experiment done with elephants. One of the pair eventually realized that it could just stand on its end of the rope and let its partner do all the pulling. Noms all around.

Leave a Comment

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *